Synaptic turnover is both stable and dynamic

The synapses in our brains that form the connections between neurons are always changing, even when nothing new needs to be learned. In our paper, we propose a simple model that can reproduce these seemingly random changes and link them with alterations associated with active learning.
Synaptic turnover is both stable and dynamic
Like

The human brain excels at various tasks, even outperforming artificial neural networks in tasks that require flexibility and generalization. Interestingly, unlike artificial networks, the synapses in the brain are subject to continuous turnover, while our brain functions are fundamentally stable1. In contrast, artificial neural networks keep their synapses fixed after training, and making even minor connectivity adjustments can yield vastly different outcomes. This raises questions about how the brain balances ongoing synaptic change while safeguarding robustness and ensuring information retention despite the turnover of its synapses. What are the features of synaptic turnover that could help achieve functional stability?

In our paper, "Linking spontaneous and stimulated spine dynamics,"2 we studied this synaptic turnover and its properties. Synapses are the microscopic "communication bridges" in your brain, where neurons exchange electrical and chemical signals to transmit information, enabling everything from thought and memory to movement and emotion. An example of how the postsynaptic side of a synapse (also known as a spine) looks like on a neuron can be seen in the zoomed-in picture below.

Fig 1: Example neuron with its dendrite and spiny protuberances called synapses. Credit Thomas Chater

When we get a learning cue (e.g., smelling a new smell or performing a new action), synapses connecting the relevant neurons are stimulated, resulting in changes in their features so that the next time this information needs to be processed, it is done faster and more efficiently, i.e., we have acquired a memory3,4. This concept is fundamental to Hebbian plasticity: "Cells that fire together wire together"5. What happens when no new memories need to be learned? Does the corresponding synapse remain constant? Well, the synapses, even in the absence of any trigger, change continuously… and this change can be quite significant. Just looking at one synapse in the microscope for approximately an hour, we can see that it grows and shrinks, thus changing the connection strength between the corresponding two neurons.1,6,7

Fig 2: Random changes that a spine can undergo in less than an hour.

To better understand the difference between stimulation-triggered and spontaneous changes, we performed experiments where we studied synapses of a neuron (without providing any stimulus) for roughly an hour. After this period, we noted (and confirmed from previous studies1,6-8) some interesting observations. 

  1. Even though individual synapses were changing a lot, the properties of the full synaptic population remained constant.

  2. If the synapse was big initially, it would be more likely to shrink in the next time frame, and vice versa; if it was small, it was more likely to grow.

  3. Synapses also liked to counteract their previous random change, i.e., when a synapse grew at one point, it would be more likely to get smaller again. In the data, we corroborated this by showing a negative correlation between synaptic size changes from one time step to the next.

Fig 3: Experimental results that refer to the three observations we saw above.

Using this information, we built a simple model that replicates the experimentally seen spontaneous behavior. For this, we needed three ingredients: Some randomness to mimic the biological diffusion of the proteins, some stability to ensure the synapses don't drift off to infinity or zero, and a component for the anti-correlation. This model, which is less detailed than molecular models used in other studies9-11, was able to recapitulate our dataset. However, the same three ingredients could also model the synaptic dynamics when a stimulus was present, i.e., when active learning occurred. 

To study this "active learning," we performed a new set of experiments where some synapses were stimulated and observed that

  1. Only the small synapses grew substantially after the stimulus.
  2. The stimulation led to a significant shift in the network, giving it higher information-carrying qualities.
  3. However, the shift itself was very short (~2 minutes). After that, the spines as a population went back to their random, spontaneous activity.
Fig 4: Figures for the observations related to "active-learning" experiments

By applying our model to this scenario, we could see that our model's randomness and stability components were key to reproducing these phenomena. What does this imply? It implies that despite its apparent randomness, the spines' spontaneous activity uses the same mechanisms as stimulated synaptic activity. Maybe spontaneous activity can be seen as the other side of the memory coin: the one that degrades memories and skills we don't use so that the brain can efficiently learn new things. By taking advantage of the random biological processes, the brain may have found a cost-effective way of freeing up space.

References:

  1. Hazan, L. & Ziv, N. E. Activity dependent and independent determinants of synaptic size diversity. J. Neurosci. 40, 2828–2848 (2020).
  2. Eggl, M.F., Chater, T.E., Petkovic, J. et al. Linking spontaneous and stimulated spine dynamics. Commun Biol 6, 930 (2023).
  3. Stevens, C. F. & Sullivan, J. Synaptic plasticity. Curr. Biol. 8, R151–R153 (1998).
  4. Magee, J. C. & Grienberger, C. Synaptic plasticity forms and functions. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 43, 95–117 (2020).
  5. Hebb, D. O. (2005). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. Psychology Press.
  6. Yasumatsu, N., Matsuzaki, M., Miyazaki, T., Noguchi, J. & Kasai, H. Principles of long-term dynamics of dendritic spines. J. Neurosci. 28, 13592–13608 (2008).
  7. Loewenstein, Y., Kuras, A. & Rumpel, S. Multiplicative dynamics underlie the emergence of the log-normal distribution of spine sizes in the neocortex in vivo. J. Neurosci. 31, 9481–9488 (2011).
  8. Ziv, N. E. & Fisher-Lavie, A. Presynaptic and postsynaptic scaffolds: dynamics fast and slow. Neuroscientist 20, 439–452 (2014).
  9. Shomar, A., Geyrhofer, L., Ziv, N. E. & Brenner, N. Cooperative stochastic binding and unbinding explain synaptic size dynamics and statistics. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13, e1005668 (2017).
  10. Bonilla-Quintana, M., Wörgötter, F., Tetzlaff, C. & Fauth, M. Modeling the shape of synaptic spines by their actin dynamics. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 12, 9 (2020).
  11. Statman, A., Kaufman, M., Minerbi, A., Ziv, N. E. & Brenner, N. Synaptic size dynamics as an effectively stochastic process. PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003846 (2014).

Please sign in or register for FREE

If you are a registered user on Research Communities by Springer Nature, please sign in

Subscribe to the Topic

Neuroscience
Life Sciences > Biological Sciences > Neuroscience

Related Collections

With collections, you can get published faster and increase your visibility.

Cell-cell communication

This Collection welcomes submissions that contribute to our understanding of cell-cell communication in multicellular organisms.

Publishing Model: Open Access

Deadline: Apr 25, 2024

RNA biology and RNA-based applications to medicine

This cross-journal Collection welcomes articles that report on RNA-driven cellular mechanisms, physiologically relevant RNA processes, and the development and characterisation of novel RNA-based medical applications for disease prevention, diagnostic and therapeutic purposes.

Publishing Model: Open Access

Deadline: Dec 27, 2023