How we conceived twins

In a recent paper we showed that dizygotic twinning in humans is the byproduct of a conditional strategy: ovulate a single ovum when young and switch to more frequent double ovulation with increasing age. Here’s how the paper came together.
Published in Ecology & Evolution
How we conceived twins
Like

Share this post

Choose a social network to share with, or copy the URL to share elsewhere

This is a representation of how your post may appear on social media. The actual post will vary between social networks

Rebecca Sear (a demographer) is the only one of the authors to have previously studied twinning in humans.  After reading her paper on twinning in a Gambian “natural fertility” population, Joe Tomkins (an evolutionary ecologist) emailed Rebecca suggesting that her idea of twinning being conditional on maternal quality could be modeled.  Joe and I (an evolutionary geneticist) had previously collaborated on studies of conditional strategies, he invited me to participate.  I had done some earlier modelling with my colleague Rick Smock (a mathematician), whom I recruited. Bob Black (an ecologist) joined Joe to write a simulation that, along with the mathematical model, would ask which would have the greatest lifetime reproductive success, single or double ovulating throughout one’s reproductive life, or a conditional strategy of starting out single ovulating, and then switching to double ovulation. 

The scientific literature on twinning is enormous. An important point of entry, was the byproduct hypothesis proposed by David Anderson as the “insurance ova hypothesis,” which views “human twins as an error occurring at low frequency in a system that compensates for embryo mortality.”  As we read more literature two things became apparent: (1) twinning is exceedingly rare, but increases with maternal age (that it then decreases was less strongly emphasized), and (2) as women age fertility decreases. We thought that the increase in twinning rate with maternal age might reflect an increase in double ovulation.  As Anderson suggested, this would serve as an adaptive offset to the decrease in prenatal survival.

In 1986, Nature published a Scientific Correspondence from Daniel Atkinson that contained a formula calculating twinning rate as a function of the probabilities of double ovulation and survival to birth. We realized we could use the formula to ask how twinning rate would be expected change with maternal age if the probability of double ovulating increased, and the probability of live birth decreased.  We found nine data sets in the literature and all showed an increase and then a decrease in twinning rate with maternal age. In general, we found that we could explain this pattern of age-dependent twinning if the probability of live birth per conception dropped from 55% at age 18 to around 10% at age 40, and the chance of double ovulation increased from around 2% to nearly 50%. 

Why would natural selection have favored such an ovulation strategy?  We constructed a simulation and mathematical model that estimated the lifetime reproductive success of women that always single ovulate, always double ovulate, or switched from single to double ovulation at different ages between the start and end of reproduction. We constructed these using Rebecca’s estimates of twin, singleton and maternal survival rates in the Gambia, and estimates of prenatal survival rates at different developmental stages from an analysis of over one million pregnancies in Denmark. The results were clear; the strategy that had the greatest reproductive success was one where women switched from single to double ovulation at about age 25.

We also found that women always double ovulating but only birthing singletons had greater lifetime reproductive success than women playing the conditional ovulation strategy, with a chance of birthing twins as a byproduct. Why this superior strategy has not evolved in humans is the subject of future research. 

A video summary of the paper can be found here.

Please sign in or register for FREE

If you are a registered user on Research Communities by Springer Nature, please sign in

Follow the Topic

Ecology
Life Sciences > Biological Sciences > Ecology