Bridging the Green Gap: When Climate Actions Go Beyond Beliefs

Our recent global study revealed surprising patterns in how liberals and conservatives act on climate change, showing that beliefs don’t always predict behavior. Surprisingly, people on both ends of the political spectrum are more alike in their climate actions than their beliefs might suggest.
Bridging the Green Gap: When Climate Actions Go Beyond Beliefs
Like

Share this post

Choose a social network to share with, or copy the URL to share elsewhere

This is a representation of how your post may appear on social media. The actual post will vary between social networks

It is generally thought that people with politically liberal beliefs accept the reality of anthropogenic climate change. You might think they would also be more likely to engage in real-world climate actions, right? However, our recent findings from a global megastudy, involving over 51,000 participants from 60 countries– one of the largest experiments conducted in climate psychology to date– challenge the straightforward link between beliefs and actions, suggesting a more complex relationship than previously thought.

The Green Gap

This phenomenon, termed the “green gap”, highlights the disparity between what people believe about climate change and their actual climate actions. Our study examined whether the political polarization evident in climate change beliefs manifests as polarization regarding climate actions, such as investing effort and time to generate tree-planting donations. The "green gap" hypothesis explores scenarios where liberals who believe strongly in climate change may not engage as expected in climate actions, while skeptical conservatives might still participate in actions that benefit the environment. 

Our study tested eleven targeted interventions developed by experts in the behavioral sciences, psychology, and beyond. Some examples include promoting scientific consensus (e.g., presenting information that 99% of experts agree on climate change facts), writing a letter to future generations (e.g., thinking about how one’s current actions impact future generations), and emphasizing collective actions (e.g., featuring examples of successful collective efforts that have solved past global issues, such as the restoration of the ozone layer). We observed worldwide polarization in climate change beliefs and policy support, with liberals generally showing higher levels of both compared to conservatives. Encouragingly, belief and policy support were relatively high overall, exceeding 75%. However, this polarization did not extend to the amount of effort and time invested in a climate-relevant task—liberals and conservatives were equally likely to engage in the tree-planting donation task.

Belief in climate change (Panel A), climate policy support (Panel B), and climate action (Panel C), as a function of self-reported political ideology (measured from 0 = liberal to 100 = conservative), in the control condition (N = 4302). Vertical lines represent 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals of the means at each level of political ideology; Fitted lines represent the best fit linear regression lines.

Conservative Actions Beyond Beliefs

These findings challenge the traditional belief-behavior congruence hypothesis, which suggests that a person's actions are directly influenced by their beliefs. Instead, the green gap suggests that factors such as cognitive biases [1], perceived personal costs [2], and social norms [3] can disrupt the direct translation of environmental beliefs into environmental action. Further, the mismatch between climate skeptics’ beliefs and behaviors has also been documented, for example, with conservative farmers adopting pro-environmental practices despite lacking belief in anthropogenic climate change [4].

So, who is to blame for this effect? Are liberals failing to put their money where their mouth is and act on their strong beliefs in climate change? Or are conservatives more inclined to engage in climate mitigation actions than their beliefs alone might suggest? Our data align more closely with the latter—conservatives acting despite their beliefs, rather than liberals not acting on theirs.

This is promising news for environmental efforts: the political resistance to accepting climate change or supporting climate policies does not necessarily translate into a behavioral resistance to engaging in at least one type of individual-level climate action. This may be driven by pragmatic environmentalism. While conservatives may not partake in climate actions (like tree-planting) primarily due to their beliefs about climate change—often polarized by media and party lines—they may engage for reasons that align with conservative values, such as preserving natural beauty [5]. Additionally, conservative skepticism towards systemic interventions, perceived as governmental overreach, motivates participation in direct actions like tree planting, which allow them to contribute positively without endorsing large-scale government policies. 

Effectiveness of Psychological Interventions

However, we are not solely dependent on existing beliefs and behaviors. While interventions like writing to future generations increased both conservative and liberal support for climate policies, emphasizing the scientific consensus spurred action only among liberals. This highlights the necessity of ideologically tailored approaches in promoting climate beliefs and behaviors. For instance, interventions that reduced psychological distance or promoted future-self continuity showed positive effects on both liberal and conservative climate beliefs and policy support. 

Interestingly, while these interventions helped bridge the belief gap, conservative actions were paradoxically reduced by several interventions that otherwise bolstered their climate beliefs or policy support. The fact that no single intervention significantly boosted conservative climate actions suggests that future research is needed to develop and test new methods to activate this demographic’s practical engagement with climate solutions, without necessarily relying on changing their underlying beliefs.

Interventions’ impacts on climate beliefs (Panel A), policy support (Panel B), and action (Panel C), split by self-reported political ideology (liberals in blue and conservatives in red; N = 51,224). Vertical lines indicate the average in the control condition for each ideological grouping. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals of the means. Upward triangles indicate significant increases, downward triangles indicate significant decreases, and circles indicate no statistically significant differences, always compared to the control.

However, it is not only political ideologies that need to be considered when tailoring intervention strategies! For example, baseline levels of belief in climate change (i.e., before an intervention is administered) have also been shown to impact the effectiveness of these interventions [6]. Additionally, factors such as national identity, gender, and level of education have all previously been identified as relevant in influencing various climate change beliefs and behaviors [7]. To help make sense of this and to easily explore the effects of our interventions across the globe, we have created a simple, user-friendly web-app that is freely available. This tool allows users to see how effective interventions are in different countries, by age groups, and by political ideologies. While we caution users not to rely on evidence from groups that are too small (e.g., sample sizes should include at least 30 people), it is a powerful, easy-to-use tool that policymakers, academics, journalists, and beyond can use to help better understand the effectiveness of the interventions in their people.

By understanding the underlying values and motivations of different groups, policymakers and activists can craft more effective and inclusive campaigns that not only increase awareness but also motivate real-world action. Rather than trying to eliminate differences in how conservatives and liberals approach climate action– differences which can be unexpectedly beneficial– we should focus on amplifying positive efforts across the spectrum.  By engaging conservatives and liberals in ways that align with their respective values—whether it's the pragmatic beauty of conservation or the urgent need for scientific action– we can enhance the collective effort to combat climate change. In doing so, we harness the best of both worlds to foster a more sustainable future for all.

References

[1] Fazio, R. H. Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 23, 75–109 (1990).

[2] Glasford, D. E. The privileged liberal principle‐implementation gap: How the personal behavior of privileged liberals contributes to social inequality. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 52, 865-885 (2022).

[3] Moser, A. K. Thinking green, buying green? Drivers of pro-environmental purchasing behavior. J. Consum. Mark. 32, 167-175 (2015).

[4] Davidson, D. J., Rollins, C., Lefsrud, L., Anders, S., & Hamann, A. Just don’t call it climate change: climate-skeptic farmer adoption of climate-mitigative practices. Environ. Res. Lett. 14, 034015 (2019).

[5] Shrubsole G. The environment and conservative values. In: Boyle D, Crompton T, Kirk M, et al. (eds) Different Politics, Same Planet: Values for Sustainable Development Beyond Left and Right. PIRC, pp.33–44 (2011).

[6] Vlasceanu, M. et al. Addressing climate change with behavioral science: a global intervention tournament in 63 countries. Sci. Adv. 10, eadj5778 (2024).

[7] Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., Bain, P. G. & Fielding, K. S. Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 6, 622–626 (2016).

Cover Image

Generated using Midjourney

Please sign in or register for FREE

If you are a registered user on Research Communities by Springer Nature, please sign in

Follow the Topic

Climate Change Mitigation
Humanities and Social Sciences > Society > Sociology > Environmental Social Sciences > Climate Change Mitigation
Social Psychology
Humanities and Social Sciences > Behavioral Sciences and Psychology > Social Psychology
Political Psychology
Humanities and Social Sciences > Behavioral Sciences and Psychology > Political Psychology

Related Collections

With collections, you can get published faster and increase your visibility.

Biology of rare genetic disorders

This cross-journal Collection between Nature Communications, Communications Biology, npj Genomic Medicine and Scientific Reports brings together research articles that provide new insights into the biology of rare genetic disorders, also known as Mendelian or monogenic disorders.

Publishing Model: Open Access

Deadline: Jan 31, 2025

Advances in catalytic hydrogen evolution

This collection encourages submissions related to hydrogen evolution catalysis, particularly where hydrogen gas is the primary product. This is a cross-journal partnership between the Energy Materials team at Nature Communications with Communications Chemistry, Communications Engineering, Communications Materials, and Scientific Reports. We seek studies covering a range of perspectives including materials design & development, catalytic performance, or underlying mechanistic understanding. Other works focused on potential applications and large-scale demonstration of hydrogen evolution are also welcome.

Publishing Model: Open Access

Deadline: Dec 31, 2024