Experimental river noise alters bird and bat abundance and behavior across a landscape

Natural noise is an underappreciated ecological niche axis
Published in Ecology & Evolution
Experimental river noise alters bird and bat abundance and behavior across a landscape
Like

Share this post

Choose a social network to share with, or copy the URL to share elsewhere

This is a representation of how your post may appear on social media. The actual post will vary between social networks

Human-made noise is drastically altering the acoustic environment, which can negatively affect animal abundance, shape biological communities, and reduce reproductive success. Yet, we do not know what mechanisms drive these patterns today nor do we understand how the selective pressures of the evolutionary past might inform our understanding of contemporary human-created noise.

A few years ago my PhD advisor, Dr. Jesse Barber, and his collaborator, Dr. Clinton Francis, came up with the idea to experimentally broadcast whitewater river noise across a landscape to see how wildlife respond to this ancient source of noise. In a natural experiment (i.e. using a naturally-existing gradient of stream noise) there are many stream variables that are confounded with noise exposure, such as flow, slope, and humidity. The experimental approach that we took, however, allowed us to tease apart the effects of noise from other habitat variables.

A whitewater river in Idaho. Photo credit: Dylan Gomes

Yet the advantages of experimental ecology do not stop there. By spectrally shifting river noise upwards in frequency at some of our experimental sites, we were also able to answer questions about mechanism of noise disturbance. The prevailing hypothesis for why many animals avoid noise is called masking. Masking occurs when noise overlaps in frequency (i.e. perceived as pitch) with a biological signal or cue. By broadcasting noise of different frequencies, we hoped to assess the role that masking of important signals or cues, such as birdsong, plays in the avoidance of noisy places.

Schematic display demonstrating how noise (shaded region) might filter out birds and bats from a location based on the sound frequencies that those taxa use, such as during communication or foraging. Figure adapted from Gomes et al. 2021 Nature Communications

To do this work, we first had to find someone who was willing to let us fill the acoustic environment with consistent, high-intensity whitewater river noise, across multiple summer seasons. Jesse spent almost an entire year making the pitch to research stations and experimental forests across the Western US and exploring public lands by foot (and with teams of llamas!) until he met Brian and Kathleen Bean of Lava Lake Ranch. The Bean’s property stretches from the Craters of the Moon National Monument to the top of the Pioneer Mountains in Central Idaho – a landscape on which they make a living raising sheep, a wilderness they love, and habitat they aim to understand and conserve via the Lava Lake Institute for Science and Conservation.

Map of field sites for experimental noise playback in the Pioneer Mountains of Idaho. Circles represent control sites where no noise playback occurred. Squares represent experimental whitewater river playback sites. Triangles represent river playback sites that were shifted upwards in frequency. Figure adapted from Gomes et al. 2021 Nature Communications

Once we selected our field sites on the ranch, we then faced the challenge of getting our nearly 4 tons (~3500 kg) of gear out to these rugged, near-roadless places. For most sites, in both years, we hiked all of our gear into the sites on our backs, and then hiked it out again at the end of the summer, before cold blizzards and avalanches could destroy our solar panels and suck our deep-cycle batteries dry. A notable exception came during the first year of the study when the county access road to some of our sites was completely washed away by massive flooding from an intense snowpack the previous winter. Without this road, the distances we needed to hike this heavy equipment more than doubled. So we hired Robert and Carla Hennefer (of Little Wood River Outfitters) and a string of mules to help us get our gear to our sites.

Hunter Cole (a co-author of the study) is organizing some of the many batteries used in the noise playback experiment. Photo credit: Dylan Gomes.
Large amounts of acoustics gear wait to be loaded into trailers before the initial trip to the field. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.
Solar panels and other gear are loaded into trailers for transport to field sites. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.
The lead author (background) and a lab technician, Nic Carlson, (foreground) carry a solar panel to a field site. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth. 
Krystie Miner and other members of the Barber Lab ford a stream packing gear into road-less sites in the Pioneer Mountains of Idaho. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.
The team is moving a covered solar panel as they prepare to pack the mules. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth. 
Dr. Cory Toth (left) and Dr. Dylan Gomes (right), both authors of this study, are fastening solar panels to a mule. Photo credit: Krystie Miner.
Robert Hennefer, a Carey, ID local and mule packer, leads the mule train across a stream. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.
Dr. Jesse Barber (the senior author of the study) is leading a mule around blown-down trees from the previous winter's blizzards. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.
Dr. Dylan Gomes, the lead author of the study, is shown here deploying a speaker early in the spring. Playback of whitewater river noise began before most birds and bats were using this high elevation mountain area. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.
One speaker in a multi-speaker array at a noise playback site. This speaker array was used to experimentally modify the soundscape of this small riparian stream into one that has the intensity of a whitewater river. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.

The work did not stop there though; we were tasked with the challenge of continuously monitoring the wildlife and their behavior at our sites. Each day this meant point-counting birds and replacing batteries on bat detectors and soundscape recorders. This amounted to 2,969 individual 3-minute counts or 150 hours of avian point counting, more than 100,000 bat sequences, and over 106,000 hours of recordings of the acoustic environment. 

Ultrasonic microphones and bat detectors were used to assess levels of bat activity across the field experiment. Over 100,000 bat passes were identified to the species level. Photo credit: Hunter Cole.
Acoustic recording units were surrounded by large wind-screens (right) and powered for two weeks at a time by batteries (in case on left). Researchers continually swapped batteries at the 60 field locations that were associated with recorders. 106,000 hours of recordings were collected and analyzed as a part of this project.

We then integrated all of this information into statistical models (i.e. generalized linear mixed-effects models) that helped us explain patterns driven by noise, while controlling for other habitat variables. We found that birds and bats both avoided loud places (or more accurately places with high sound levels). Birds in our study area avoided noise that overlaps in frequency with their song – so their communication was masked by noise. Interestingly this pattern dissipated at high sound levels, meaning that birds were avoiding the noise for other reasons when the noise was really intense. Bats avoided areas with higher frequency noise, but since bats have much higher echolocation frequencies (relative to river noise), we were not able to explicitly test masking of vocalizations.

Communication, however, is not the only thing that might be disrupted by noise. Foraging is a task that is necessary to survival for all animals. During this study we glued more than 700 clay caterpillars to willow branches to measure changes in bird foraging, and we found that these caterpillars were much less often attacked when the acoustic environment was loud – even though the foraging task was entirely visual!

Seven-hundred and twenty clay caterpillars were used in the study to assess bird foraging activity. The imprint of a bird's beak can be seen near the top of the arch of this clay caterpillar. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.

For bats, we built foraging tests that helped us assess ‘strategy switching’ during hunting. Some bats are able to hunt with either of two hunting strategies; these strategies are called passive listening and active listening. Passive listening is when the animal finds its prey simply by locating sounds coming from the prey. These bats listen to the footsteps and songs of insects like beetles or crickets. Since these sounds are lower frequency, we would expect them to be masked by river noise. Active listening, on the other hand, is echolocation. That is, bats produce extremely high frequency sonar calls that allow them to detect flying insects as the sound of their call echoes off their prey. This high frequency sonar should not be masked by river noise. So we might expect bats to switch between these two foraging strategies in the presence of noise – and that is exactly what we found!

Motor-driven fluttering insect wings (right and inset) and ultrasonic bat detectors (left) were used to assess shifts in bat foraging. Photo credit: Dr. Cory Toth.

Our research highlights that the acoustic environment is important in determining where animals live and how they behave. It seems that the sum of all the sounds in any given place, form an important axis in determining where species can persist. On top of adding another dimension to niche theory, our work argues that noise is a disrupting force that many animals have not been able to circumvent despite eons of selection. This selection might have been too diffuse for many as naturally loud environments (crashing surf, windswept ridges, raging rivers) are substantially rarer than the anthropogenic soundscapes that now nearly dominant the entire planet. The evolutionary lens of our work only stands to amplify the call for human-caused noise management.

 

The lead author (Dr. Dylan Gomes) hiking between two field sites, while looking down at other nearby sites on Iron Mine Creek. Photo credit: Hunter Cole.

Please sign in or register for FREE

If you are a registered user on Research Communities by Springer Nature, please sign in

Follow the Topic

Ecology
Life Sciences > Biological Sciences > Ecology

Related Collections

With collections, you can get published faster and increase your visibility.

Biology of rare genetic disorders

This cross-journal Collection between Nature Communications, Communications Biology, npj Genomic Medicine and Scientific Reports brings together research articles that provide new insights into the biology of rare genetic disorders, also known as Mendelian or monogenic disorders.

Publishing Model: Open Access

Deadline: Jan 31, 2025

Advances in catalytic hydrogen evolution

This collection encourages submissions related to hydrogen evolution catalysis, particularly where hydrogen gas is the primary product. This is a cross-journal partnership between the Energy Materials team at Nature Communications with Communications Chemistry, Communications Engineering, Communications Materials, and Scientific Reports. We seek studies covering a range of perspectives including materials design & development, catalytic performance, or underlying mechanistic understanding. Other works focused on potential applications and large-scale demonstration of hydrogen evolution are also welcome.

Publishing Model: Open Access

Deadline: Dec 31, 2024